Posted but not written by: Louis Sheehan
As the 2008 U.S. presidential election
approaches, tens of millions of voters have to make up their minds. They face
the task of sifting through media reports, televised debates, political
advertisements, campaign literature and conversations with family and friends
to identify a candidate who best reflects their political views.
That just may be too much to ask,
though. As political scientists have long lamented, the general public knows
depressingly little about politics. Most Americans can identify the president
but barely half know the name of even one cabinet member and only one-third
correctly identify their two U.S. senators or their congressional
representative. In surveys, roughly half of registered voters display little
understanding of how government works or of current political issues.
Even if a voter knew enough to evaluate
each presidential candidate’s positions on diverse issues, he or she would
still need to tally pros and cons on those issues for each candidate and
determine who most deserved support. Decision researchers in various fields
have long favored this exhaustive, coldly logical approach, even if only as an
ideal that less methodical thinkers should strive for.
Yet according to many psychologists,
people will never think that way. We shun rationality and seek as little
information as possible when making judgments, the experts assert. Instead,
individuals use strategic shortcuts, also known as rules of thumb or
heuristics, to decide. The latter term, of Greek origin, means “serving to find
out or discover.” Heuristics require minimal mental effort but prompt irrational
and biased judgments — or at least so say some psychologists.
Political scientists generally assume
just the opposite. They regard heuristics as tools for the average citizen to
fashion reasonably accurate political judgments out of sparse civic knowledge.
A recent experimental innovation
promises to better illuminate heuristics’ strengths and weaknesses. Researchers
now can track how volunteers decide whom to vote for during mock presidential
election campaigns. Results so far indicate that well-informed voters employ
heuristics better than they do extensive information analyses to select a
candidate who best reflects their own views. In contrast, poorly informed
voters experience problems in picking appropriate candidates, especially when
using rules of thumb.
In general, rational folk who seek as
much information as possible about candidates’ positions on many different
issues tend to make poorer decisions about whom to support in mock campaigns
than do those who follow simple heuristics. These rules of thumb include
choosing candidates based on what political party they belong to or which
organizations endorse them.
“At least in politics, more
information does not always result in better decisions,” says political
scientist Richard Lau of Rutgers University in New Brunswick, N.J. “In fact, it
often results in worse decisions.”
Other new research suggests that
heuristics based solely on certain emotional reactions to candidates, such as
admiration and contempt, also guide voting decisions surprisingly well.
12-minute campaigns
Although political scientists
typically use surveys to examine voters’ attitudes about political issues and
candidates, Lau and colleague David Redlawsk of the University of Iowa in Iowa
City take a different approach. They use computers to model campaigns and track
how people actually decide whom to vote for in mock elections.
Lau and Redlawsk revised the classic
“information board” that has long been used in psychology and marketing to
study decision making. An information board looks much like the game board for
the television show Jeopardy, with a matrix of columns and rows of boxes that
conceal information. Columns on the board are headed by various alternatives,
such as a series of political candidates. Rows are labeled with different
attributes, such as experience and stands on issues.
In the updated version, volunteers
uncover information that they want to learn by clicking a box on the screen.
Researchers record what information gets examined, the order in which it’s
retrieved and how long it’s perused. Over the past seven years, the two
investigators’ findings based on the method have stirred much interest at
political science conferences.
Lau and Redlawsk’s “dynamic
process-tracing” method uses the information board format to mimic the
overwhelming flow of information during presidential campaigns. This approach
features a mock primary campaign with six candidates, two Democrats and four
Republicans or vice versa, followed by a general election campaign between each
party’s nominee. Volunteers register with a party, vote in that party’s primary
and then cast ballots in the general election.
The primary campaign lasts about 20
minutes. The general election unfolds over 12 minutes.
During a campaign, columns of boxes on
what looks like an information board scroll down a computer screen and
disappear, replaced by others at the top of the screen. Participants thus have
access to only a fraction of the total information pool at any one time. As in
real campaigns, some types of candidate information, such as poll results,
appear more often than others do, such as endorsements and issue statements.
access
Voting Behavior and Emotional
HeuristicsEmotions, specifically admiration and contempt for a politician,
mirrored voters' stated preference better than political party affiliation in a
study of 70 voters before the 2004 election, suggesting that these emotions are
often used as shortcuts when choosing a candidate. Click on the picture to view
a larger version.J. Korenblat (source: Wang, et al., J. Behavioral Decision
Making, 2008
At regular intervals, a 20-second
political advertisement from one of the candidates takes over the computer
screen.
Similar to most voters, no one in the
study can read and consider every bit of information presented during these
mock campaigns, much less compare candidates on every political attribute.
If a participant employs a particular
heuristic, such as paying special attention to which groups endorse different
candidates, then Lau and Redlawsk can see whether that person consistently
clicks on endorsements during a campaign.
Before the mock campaign, researchers
survey each volunteer’s political attitudes to determine the candidate that
most closely aligns with each volunteer’s views — thus the best voting choice.
In a groundbreaking 2001 study that
launched the real-time analysis of how people make voting choices, Lau and
Redlawsk found that nearly all of 657 eligible voters, ages 18 to 84, used
heuristics at least some of the time in determining which mock candidate to
support. Available shortcuts included relying on a candidate’s party
affiliation, making assumptions about a candidate’s ideology based on party
affiliation, checking candidate endorsements, tracking poll leaders and judging
candidates based on their physical appearance in photographs.
Using shortcuts — especially the
tracking of endorsements — allowed most politically sophisticated volunteers,
as determined in a survey, to choose and vote for the candidate who best represented
their views. That proportion dipped to a bare majority among those who didn’t
use heuristics.
Unlike informed voters, politically
naïve volunteers usually failed to vote in their own best interests if they
used heuristics. Uninformed participants did better when they avoided using
rules of thumb, identifying the best-suited candidate about half the time.
Posted but not written by: Louis Sheehan
No comments:
Post a Comment